تصنيف العواطف

(تم التحويل من Emotion classification)
Colored intaglio prints by Charles Le Brun and J. Pass depicting the facial expressions of sixteen emotions

تصنيف العواطف Emotion classification ، the means by which one may distinguish or contrast one emotion from another, is a contested issue in emotion research and in affective science. Researchers have approached the classification of emotions from one of two fundamental viewpoints:

  1. that emotions are discrete and fundamentally different constructs
  2. that emotions can be characterized on a dimensional basis in groupings

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Emotions as discrete categories

In discrete emotion theory, all humans are thought to have an innate set of basic emotions that are cross-culturally recognizable. These basic emotions are described as "discrete" because they are believed to be distinguishable by an individual's facial expression and biological processes.[1] Theorists have conducted studies to determine which emotions are basic. A popular example is Paul Ekman and his colleagues' cross-cultural study of 1992, in which they concluded that the six basic emotions are anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise.[2] Ekman explains that there are particular characteristics attached to each of these emotions, allowing them to be expressed in varying degrees. Each emotion acts as a discrete category rather than an individual emotional state.[3]


Basicality debate

Humans' subjective experience is that emotions are clearly recognizable in ourselves and others. This apparent ease of recognition has led to the identification of a number of emotions that are said to be basic, and universal among all people. However, a debate among experts has questioned this understanding of what emotions are. There has been recent discussion of the progression on the different views of emotion over the years.[4]

On "basic emotion" accounts, activation of an emotion, such as anger, sadness, or fear, is "triggered" by the brain's appraisal of a stimulus or event with respect to the perceiver's goals or survival. In particular, the function, expression, and meaning of different emotions are hypothesized to be biologically distinct from one another. A theme common to many basic emotions theories is that there should be functional signatures that distinguish different emotions: we should be able to tell what emotion a person is feeling by looking at his or her brain activity and/or physiology. Furthermore, knowledge of what the person is seeing or the larger context of the eliciting event should not be necessary to deduce what the person is feeling from observing the biological signatures.[3]

On "constructionist" accounts, the emotion a person feels in response to a stimulus or event is "constructed" from more elemental biological and psychological ingredients. Two hypothesized ingredients are "core affect" (characterized by, e.g., hedonic valence and physiological arousal) and conceptual knowledge (such as the semantic meaning of the emotion labels themselves, e.g., the word "anger"). A theme common to many constructionist theories is that different emotions do not have specific locations in the nervous system or distinct physiological signatures, and that context is central to the emotion a person feels because of the accessibility of different concepts afforded by different contexts.[5]

Semantically distinct emotions

Eugene Bann proposed a theory that people transmit their understanding of emotions through the language they use that surrounds mentioned emotion keywords. He posits that the more distinct language is used to express a certain emotion, then the more distinct the perception (including proprioception) of that emotion is, and thus more basic. This allows us to select the dimensions best representing the entire spectrum of emotion. Coincidentally, it was found that Ekman's (1972) basic emotion set, arguably the most frequently used for classifying emotions, is the most semantically distinct.[6]

Dimensional models of emotion

For both theoretical and practical reasons researchers define emotions according to one or more dimensions. In his philosophical treatise, The Passions of the Soul, Descartes defines and investigates the six primary passions (wonder, love, hate, desire, joy, and sadness). Wilhelm Max Wundt, the father of modern psychology, proposed in 1897 that emotions can be described by three dimensions: "pleasurable versus unpleasurable", "arousing or subduing" and "strain or relaxation".[7] In 1954 Harold Schlosberg named three dimensions of emotion: "pleasantness–unpleasantness", "attention–rejection" and "level of activation".[8]

Dimensional models of emotion attempt to conceptualize human emotions by defining where they lie in two or three dimensions. Most dimensional models incorporate valence and arousal or intensity dimensions. Dimensional models of emotion suggest that a common and interconnected neurophysiological system is responsible for all affective states.[9] These models contrast theories of basic emotion, which propose that different emotions arise from separate neural systems.[9] Several dimensional models of emotion have been developed, though there are just a few that remain as the dominant models currently accepted by most.[10] The two-dimensional models that are most prominent are the circumplex model, the vector model, and the Positive Activation – Negative Activation (PANA) model.[10]

Circumplex model

The circumplex model of emotion was developed by James Russell.[11] This model suggests that emotions are distributed in a two-dimensional circular space, containing arousal and valence dimensions. Arousal represents the vertical axis and valence represents the horizontal axis, while the center of the circle represents a neutral valence and a medium level of arousal.[10] In this model, emotional states can be represented at any level of valence and arousal, or at a neutral level of one or both of these factors. Circumplex models have been used most commonly to test stimuli of emotion words, emotional facial expressions, and affective states.[12]

Russell and Lisa Feldman Barrett describe their modified circumplex model as representative of core affect, or the most elementary feelings that are not necessarily directed toward anything. Different prototypical emotional episodes, or clear emotions that are evoked or directed by specific objects, can be plotted on the circumplex, according to their levels of arousal and pleasure.[13]

Vector model

The vector model of emotion appeared in 1992.[14] This two-dimensional model consists of vectors that point in two directions, representing a "boomerang" shape. The model assumes that there is always an underlying arousal dimension, and that valence determines the direction in which a particular emotion lies. For example, a positive valence would shift the emotion up the top vector and a negative valence would shift the emotion down the bottom vector.[10] In this model, high arousal states are differentiated by their valence, whereas low arousal states are more neutral and are represented near the meeting point of the vectors. Vector models have been most widely used in the testing of word and picture stimuli.[12]

Positive activation – negative activation (PANA) model

The positive activation – negative activation (PANA) or "consensual" model of emotion, originally created by Watson and Tellegen in 1985,[15] suggests that positive affect and negative affect are two separate systems. Similar to the vector model, states of higher arousal tend to be defined by their valence, and states of lower arousal tend to be more neutral in terms of valence.[10] In the PANA model, the vertical axis represents low to high positive affect and the horizontal axis represents low to high negative affect. The dimensions of valence and arousal lay at a 45-degree rotation over these axes.[15]

Plutchik's model

Robert Plutchik offers a three-dimensional model that is a hybrid of both basic-complex categories and dimensional theories. It arranges emotions in concentric circles where inner circles are more basic and outer circles more complex. Notably, outer circles are also formed by blending the inner circle emotions. Plutchik's model, as Russell's, emanates from a circumplex representation, where emotional words were plotted based on similarity.[16] There are numerous emotions, which appear in several intensities and can be combined in various ways to form emotional "dyads".[17][18][19][20][21]

PAD emotional state model

The PAD emotional state model is a psychological model developed by Albert Mehrabian and James A. Russell to describe and measure emotional states. PAD uses three numerical dimensions to represent all emotions.[22][23] The PAD dimensions are Pleasure, Arousal and Dominance.

The Pleasure-Displeasure Scale measures how pleasant an emotion may be. For instance both anger and fear are unpleasant emotions, and score high on the displeasure scale. However joy is a pleasant emotion.[22]

The Arousal-Nonarousal Scale measures how energized or soporific one feels. It is not the intensity of the emotion—for grief and depression can be low arousal intense feelings. While both anger and rage are unpleasant emotions, rage has a higher intensity or a higher arousal state. However boredom, which is also an unpleasant state, has a low arousal value.[22]

The Dominance-Submissiveness Scale represents the controlling and dominant nature of the emotion. For instance while both fear and anger are unpleasant emotions, anger is a dominant emotion, while fear is a submissive emotion.[22]


. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Criticisms

Cultural considerations

Ethnographic and cross-cultural studies of emotions have shown the variety of ways in which emotions differ with cultures. Because of these differences, many cross-cultural psychologists and anthropologists challenge the idea of universal classifications of emotions altogether.

Cultural differences have been observed in the way in which emotions are valued, expressed, and regulated. The social norms for emotions, such as the frequency with or circumstances in which they are expressed, also vary drastically.[24][25] For example, the demonstration of anger is encouraged by Kaluli people, but condemned by Utku Inuit.[26] The largest piece of evidence that disputes the universality of emotions is language. Differences within languages directly correlate to differences in emotion taxonomy. Languages differ in that they categorize emotions based on different components. Some may categorize by event types whereas others categorize by action readiness. Furthermore, emotion taxonomies vary due to the differing implications emotions have in different languages.[24] That being said, not all English words have equivalents in all other languages and vice versa, indicating that there are words for emotions present in some languages but not in others.[27] Emotions such as the schadenfreude in German and saudade in Portuguese are commonly expressed in emotions in their respective languages, but lack an English equivalent. Some languages do not differentiate between emotions that are considered to be the basic emotions in English. For instance, certain African languages have one word for both anger and sadness, and others for shame and fear. There is ethnographic evidence that even challenges the universality of the category "emotions" because certain cultures lack a specific word relating to the English word "emotions".[25]

Lists of emotions

Humans experience emotion, with evidence used that they influence action, thoughts and behavior. Emotions are categorized into various affects, which correspond to the current situation.[28] An affect is the range of feeling experienced.[29] Both positive and negative emotions are needed in our daily lives.[30]

Many theories of emotion have been proposed,[31] with contrasting views.[32]

Basic emotions

  • William James in 1890 proposed four basic emotions: fear, grief, love, and rage, based on bodily involvement.[33]
  • Paul Ekman identified six basic emotions: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise.[34] Wallace V. Friesen and Phoebe C. Ellsworth worked with him on the same basic structure.[35] The emotions can be linked to facial expressions. In the 1990s, Ekman proposed an expanded list of basic emotions, including a range of positive and negative emotions that are not all encoded in facial muscles.[36] The newly included emotions are: Amusement, Contempt, Contentment, Embarrassment, Excitement, Guilt, Pride in achievement, Relief, Satisfaction, Sensory pleasure, and Shame.[36]
  • Richard and Bernice Lazarus in 1996 expanded the list to 15 emotions: aesthetic experience, anger, anxiety, compassion, depression, envy, fright, gratitude, guilt, happiness, hope, jealousy, love, pride, relief, sadness, and shame, in the book Passion and Reason.[37][38]
  • Researchers[39] at University of California, Berkeley identified 27 categories of emotion: admiration, adoration, aesthetic appreciation, amusement, anger, anxiety, awe, awkwardness, boredom, calmness, confusion, craving, disgust, empathic pain, entrancement, excitement, fear, horror, interest, joy, nostalgia, relief, romance, sadness, satisfaction, sexual desire and surprise.[40] This was based on 2185 short videos intended to elicit a certain emotion. These were then modeled onto a "map" of emotions.[41]

Contrasting basic emotions

A 2009 review[42] of theories of emotion identifies and contrasts fundamental emotions according to three key criteria for mental experiences that:

  1. have a strongly motivating subjective quality like pleasure or pain;
  2. are a response to some event or object that is either real or imagined;
  3. motivate particular kinds of behavior.

The combination of these attributes distinguishes emotions from sensations, feelings and moods.

Kind of emotion Positive emotions Negative emotions
Related to object properties Interest, curiosity, enthusiasm Indifference, habituation, boredom
Attraction, desire, admiration Aversion, disgust, revulsion
Surprise, amusement Alarm, panic
Future appraisal Hope, excitement Fear, anxiety, dread
Event-related Gratitude, thankfulness Anger, rage
Joy, elation, triumph, jubilation Sorrow, grief
Patience Frustration, restlessness
Contentment Discontentment, disappointment
Self-appraisal Humility, modesty Pride, arrogance
Social Charity Avarice, greed, miserliness, envy, jealousy
Sympathy Cruelty
Cathected Love Hate

HUMAINE's proposal for EARL

The emotion annotation and representation language (EARL) proposed by the Human-Machine Interaction Network on Emotion (HUMAINE) classifies 48 emotions.[43]


. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Parrott's emotions by groups

A tree-structured list of emotions was described in Shaver et al. (1987),[44] and also featured in Parrott (2001).[45]

Primary emotion Secondary emotion Tertiary emotion
Love Affection Adoration • Fondness • Liking • Attraction • Caring • Tenderness • Compassion • Sentimentality
Lust/Sexual desire Desire • Passion • Infatuation
Longing Longing
Joy Cheerfulness Amusement • Bliss • Gaiety • Glee • Jolliness • Joviality • Joy • Delight • Enjoyment • Gladness • Happiness • Jubilation • Elation • Satisfaction • Ecstasy • Euphoria
Zest Enthusiasm • Zeal • Excitement • Thrill • Exhilaration
Contentment Pleasure
Pride Triumph
Optimism Eagerness • Hope
Enthrallment Enthrallment • Rapture
Relief Relief
Surprise Surprise Amazement • Astonishment
Anger Irritability Aggravation • Agitation • Annoyance • Grouchy • Grumpy • Crosspatch
Exasperation Frustration
Rage Anger • Outrage • Fury • Wrath • Hostility • Ferocity • Bitterness • Hatred • Scorn • Spite • Vengefulness • Dislike • Resentment
Disgust Revulsion • Contempt • Loathing
Envy Jealousy
Torment Torment
Sadness Suffering Agony • Anguish • Hurt
Sadness Depression • Despair • Gloom • Glumness • Unhappiness • Grief • Sorrow • Woe • Misery • Melancholy
Disappointment Dismay • Displeasure
Shame Guilt • Regret • Remorse
Neglect Alienation • Defeatism • Dejection • Embarrassment • Homesickness • Humiliation • Insecurity • Insult • Isolation • Loneliness • Rejection
Sympathy Pity • Mono no aware • Sympathy
Fear Horror Alarm • Shock • Fear • Fright • Horror • Terror • Panic • Hysteria • Mortification
Nervousness Anxiety • Suspense • Uneasiness • Apprehension (fear) • Worry • Distress • Dread

عجلة العواطف لپلوتشيك

  Anticipation
  Joy
  Trust
  Fear
  Surprise
  Sadness
  Disgust
  Anger
The primary, secondary and tertiary dyads.
عجلة العواطف

In 1980, Robert Plutchik diagrammed a wheel of eight emotions: joy, trust, fear, surprise, sadness, disgust, anger and anticipation, inspired by his Ten Postulates.[46][47] Plutchik also theorized twenty-four "Primary", "Secondary", and "Tertiary" dyads (feelings composed of two emotions).[48][49][50][51][52][53][54] The wheel emotions can be paired in four groups:

Primary dyad = one petal apart = Love = Joy + Trust
Secondary dyad = two petals apart = Envy = Sadness + Anger
Tertiary dyad = three petals apart = Shame = Fear + Disgust
Opposite emotions = four petals apart = AnticipationSurprise

There are also triads, emotions formed from 3 primary emotions, though Plutchik never describes in any detail what the triads might be.[55] This leads to a combination of 24 dyads and 32 triads, making 56 emotions at 1 intensity level.[56] Emotions can be mild or intense;[57] for example, distraction is a mild form of surprise, and rage is an intense form of anger. The kinds of relation between each pair of emotions are:

Emotions and opposites
Mild emotion Mild opposite Basic emotion Basic opposite Intense emotion Intense opposite
Serenity Pensiveness, Gloominess Joy, Cheerfulness Sadness, Dejection Ecstasy, Elation Grief, Sorrow
Acceptance, Tolerance Boredom, Dislike Trust Disgust, Aversion Admiration, Adoration Loathing, Revulsion
Apprehension, Dismay Annoyance, Irritation Fear, Fright Anger, Hostility Terror, Panic Rage, Fury
Distraction, Uncertainty Interest, Attentiveness Surprise Anticipation, Expectancy Amazement, Astonishment Vigilance
Dyads (Combinations)
Human feelings Emotions Opposite feelings Emotions
Optimism, Courage Anticipation + Joy Disapproval, Disappointment Surprise + Sadness
Hope, Fatalism Anticipation + Trust Unbelief, Shock Surprise + Disgust
Anxiety, Dread Anticipation + Fear Outrage, Hate Surprise + Anger
Love, Friendliness Joy + Trust Remorse, Misery Sadness + Disgust
Guilt, Excitement Joy + Fear Envy, Sullenness Sadness + Anger
Delight, Doom Joy + Surprise Pessimism Sadness + Anticipation
Submission, Modesty Trust + Fear Contempt, Scorn Disgust + Anger
Curiosity Trust + Surprise Cynicism Disgust + Anticipation
Sentimentality, Resignation Trust + Sadness Morbidness, Derisiveness Disgust + Joy
Awe, Alarm Fear + Surprise Aggressiveness, Vengeance Anger + Anticipation
Despair Fear + Sadness Pride, Victory Anger + Joy
Shame, Prudishness Fear + Disgust Dominance Anger + Trust
Opposite combinations[51]
Human feelings Emotions
Bittersweetness Joy + Sadness
Ambivalence Trust + Disgust
Frozenness Fear + Anger
Confusion Surprise + Anticipation

Similar emotions in the wheel are adjacent to each other.[58] Anger, Anticipation, Joy, and Trust are positive in valence, while Fear, Surprise, Sadness, and Disgust are negative in valence. Anger is classified as a "positive" emotion because it involves "moving toward" a goal,[59] while surprise is negative because it is a violation of someone's territory.[60] The emotion dyads each have half-opposites and exact opposites:[61]

Anticipation, Joy, Surprise, Sadness
+ Sadness Joy
Anticipation Pessimism Optimism
Surprise Disapproval Delight
Joy, Trust, Sadness, Disgust
+ Disgust Trust
Joy Morbidness Love
Sadness Remorse Sentimentality
Trust, Fear, Disgust, Anger
+ Fear Anger
Trust Submission Dominance
Disgust Shame Contempt
Fear, Surprise, Anger, Anticipation
+ Surprise Anticipation
Anger Outrage Aggressiveness
Fear Awe Anxiety
Trust, Surprise, Disgust, Anticipation
+ Surprise Anticipation
Trust Curiosity Hope
Disgust Unbelief Cynicism
Joy, Fear, Sadness, Anger
+ Fear Anger
Joy Guilt Pride
Sadness Despair Envy

Six emotion axes

MIT researchers [62] published a paper titled "An Affective Model of Interplay Between Emotions and Learning: Reengineering Educational Pedagogy—Building a Learning Companion" that lists six axes of emotions with different opposite emotions, and different emotions coming from ranges.[62]

Emotional flow
Axis -1.0 -0.5 0 0 +0.5 +1.0
AnxietyConfidence Anxiety Worry Discomfort Comfort Hopeful Confident
BoredomFascination Ennui Boredom Indifference Interest Curiosity Intrigue
FrustrationEuphoria Frustration Puzzlement Confusion Insight Enlightenment Epiphany
DispiritedEncouraged Dispirited Disappointed Dissatisfied Satisfied Thrilled Enthusiastic
TerrorEnchantment Terror Dread Apprehension Calm Anticipatory Excited
HumiliationPride Humiliated Embarrassed Self-conscious Pleased Satisfied Proud

They also made a model labeling phases of learning emotions.[62]

Negative Affect Positive Affect
Constructive Learning Disappointment, Puzzlement, Confusion Awe, Satisfaction, Curiosity
Un-learning Frustration, Discard,

Misconceptions

Hopefulness, Fresh research

The Book of Human Emotions

Tiffany Watt Smith listed 154 different worldwide emotions and feelings.[63]

Mapping facial expressions

Scientists map twenty-one different facial emotions[65][66] expanded from Paul Ekman's six basic emotions of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise:

Happy Sad Fearful Angry Surprised Disgusted
HappilySurprised HappilyDisgusted
SadlyFearful SadlyAngry SadlySurprised SadlyDisgusted
Appalled FearfullyAngry FearfullySurprised FearfullyDisgusted
Awed AngrilySurprised AngrilyDisgusted
Hatred DisgustedlySurprised

Atlas of emotions

The Dalai Lama made a website based on the emotions of enjoyment, disgust, anger, fear and sadness with the help of Paul Ekman.[67][68] The emotions were similar to the ones found in Inside Out, a film that Paul Ekman advised.[69]

Emotion and stress

Emotions and stress are connected, so stressful situations produce emotion. Environments that make stress also make emotions.[70]

Emotion Dynamics

Researchers distinguish several emotion dynamics, most commonly how intense (mean level), variable (fluctuations), inert (temporal dependency), instable (magnitude of moment-to-moment fluctuations), or differentiated someone's emotions are (the specificity of granularity of emotions), and whether and how an emotion augments or blunts other emotions.[71] Meta-analytic reviews show systematic developmental changes in emotion dynamics throughout childhood and adolescence and substantial between-person differences.[71]

See also

Bibliography

  • Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expression of emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press: pp. 207–283.
  • Ekman, P. (1992). "An argument for basic emotions". Cognition and Emotion. 6 (3): 169–200. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.454.1984. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
  • Ekman, P. (1999). Basic Emotions. In T. Dalgleish and T. Power (Eds.) The Handbook of Cognition and Emotion Pp. 45–60. Sussex, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
  • Fontaine, J.; Scherer, KR; Roesch, EB; Ellsworth, PC (2007). "The world of emotions is not two-dimensional". Psychological Science. 18 (12): 1050–1057. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1031.3706. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02024.x. PMID 18031411. S2CID 1779061.
  • Koelsch, S.; Jacobs, AM.; Menninghaus, W.; Liebal, K.; Klann-Delius, G.; von Scheve, C.; Gebauer, G. (2015). "The quartet theory of human emotions: An integrative and neurofunctional model". Phys Life Rev. 13: 1–27. Bibcode:2015PhLRv..13....1K. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2015.03.001. PMID 25891321.

Notes and references

  1. ^ Colombetti, Giovanna (August 2009). "From affect programs to dynamical discrete emotions". Philosophical Psychology. 22 (4): 407–425. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.728.9666. doi:10.1080/09515080903153600. S2CID 40157414.
  2. ^ Ekman, Paul (January 1992). "Facial Expressions of Emotion: New Findings, New Questions". Psychological Science. 3 (1): 34–38. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00253.x. S2CID 9274447.
  3. ^ أ ب Ekman, Paul (1992). "An Argument for Basic Emotions". Cognition and Emotion. 6 (3/4): 169–200. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.454.1984. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
  4. ^ Gendron, Maria; Barrett, Lisa Feldman (October 2009). "Reconstructing the Past: A Century of Ideas About Emotion in Psychology". Emotion Review. 1 (4): 316–339. doi:10.1177/1754073909338877. PMC 2835158. PMID 20221412.
  5. ^ Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2006). "Solving the Emotion Paradox: Categorization and the Experience of Emotion". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (1): 20–46. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2. PMID 16430327. S2CID 7750265.
  6. ^ Bann, E. Y.; Bryson, J. J. (2014). "The Conceptualisation of Emotion Qualia: Semantic Clustering of Emotional Tweets" (PDF). Proceedings of the 13th Neural Computation and Psychology Workshop: 249–263. doi:10.1142/9789814458849_0019. ISBN 978-981-4458-83-2.
  7. ^ W.M. Wundt, Outlines of Psychology. (1897). In: Classics in the history of psychology. http://psychclassics.asu.edu/index.htm Archived 2001-02-24 at the Wayback Machine, York University 2010, Toronto.
  8. ^ Schlosberg, H. (1954). "Three dimensions of emotion". Psychological Review. 61 (2): 81–8. doi:10.1037/h0054570. PMID 13155714. S2CID 27914497.
  9. ^ أ ب Posner, Jonathan; Russell, J.A.; Peterson, B. S. (2005). "The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology". Development and Psychopathology. 17 (3): 715–734. doi:10.1017/s0954579405050340. PMC 2367156. PMID 16262989.
  10. ^ أ ب ت ث ج Rubin, D. C.; Talerico, J.M. (2009). "A comparison of dimensional models of emotion". Memory. 17 (8): 802–808. doi:10.1080/09658210903130764. PMC 2784275. PMID 19691001.
  11. ^ Russell, James (1980). "A circumplex model of affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 39 (6): 1161–1178. doi:10.1037/h0077714. hdl:10983/22919.
  12. ^ أ ب Remington, N. A.; Fabrigar, L. R.; Visser, P. S. (2000). "Re-examining the circumplex model of affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (2): 286–300. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.286. PMID 10948981.
  13. ^ Russell, James; Feldman Barrett, Lisa (1999). "Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76 (5): 805–819. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805. PMID 10353204.
  14. ^ Bradley, M. M.; Greenwald, M. K.; Petry, M.C.; Lang, P. J. (1992). "Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 18 (2): 379–390. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.379. PMID 1532823.
  15. ^ أ ب Watson, D.; Tellegen, A. (1985). "Toward a consensual structure of mood". Psychological Bulletin. 98 (2): 219–235. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219. PMID 3901060.
  16. ^ Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  17. ^ Plutchik, Robert (16 September 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. p. 110. ISBN 9780819182869. Retrieved 16 September 2017 – via Google Books.
  18. ^ Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  19. ^ "Robert Plutchik's Psychoevolutionary Theory of Basic Emotions" (PDF). Adliterate.com. Retrieved 2017-06-05.
  20. ^ Jonathan Turner (1 June 2000). On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry Into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford University Press. p. 76. ISBN 978-0-8047-6436-0.
  21. ^ Atifa Athar; M. Saleem Khan; Khalil Ahmed; Aiesha Ahmed; Nida Anwar (June 2011). "A Fuzzy Inference System for Synergy Estimation of Simultaneous Emotion Dynamics in Agents". International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research. 2 (6).
  22. ^ أ ب ت ث Mehrabian, Albert (1980). Basic dimensions for a general psychological theory. pp. 39–53. ISBN 978-0-89946-004-8.
  23. ^ Bales, Robert Freed (2001). Social interaction systems: theory and measurement. pp. 139–140. ISBN 978-0-7658-0872-1.
  24. ^ أ ب Mesquita, Batja; Nico Frijda (September 1992). "Cultural variations in emotions: a review". Psychological Bulletin. 112 (2): 179–204. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.179. PMID 1454891.
  25. ^ أ ب Russell, James (1991). "Culture and Categorization of Emotions" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 110 (3): 426–450. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426. PMID 1758918. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-04. Retrieved 15 December 2015.
  26. ^ Eid, Michael; Ed Diener (November 2001). "Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter- and intranational differences". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (5): 869–885. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.869. PMID 11708563.قالب:Paywall
  27. ^ Wierzbicka, Anna (September 1986). "Human Emotions: Universal or Culture-Specific?". American Anthropologist. 88 (3): 584–594. doi:10.1525/aa.1986.88.3.02a00030. JSTOR 679478.قالب:Paywall
  28. ^ Lisa Feldman Barrett (2006). "Solving the Emotion Paradox : Categorization and the Experience of Emotion". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (1): 20–46. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2. PMID 16430327. S2CID 7750265.
  29. ^ "Emotions and Moods" (PDF). Catalogue.pearsoned.co.uk. Retrieved 20 October 2017.
  30. ^ Parrott, W. Gerrod (27 January 2014). The Positive Side of Negative Emotions. Guilford Publications. ISBN 9781462513338. Retrieved 19 December 2018 – via Google Books.
  31. ^ "Comparing The 5 Theories of Emotion – Brain Blogger". Mind. os–IX (34): 188–205. 1884. doi:10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188. Retrieved 23 November 2017.
  32. ^ Candland, Douglas (23 November 2017). Emotion. iUniverse. ISBN 9780595270262. Retrieved 23 November 2017 – via Google Books.
  33. ^ James, William (1 April 2007). The Principles of Psychology. Cosimo, Inc. ISBN 9781602063136. Retrieved 20 October 2017 – via Google Books.
  34. ^ Handel, Steven (2011-05-24). "Classification of Emotions". Retrieved 30 April 2012.
  35. ^ "Are There Basic Emotions?" (PDF). Paulekam.com. Retrieved 20 October 2017.
  36. ^ أ ب Ekman, Paul (1999), "Basic Emotions", in Dalgleish, T; Power, M, Handbook of Cognition and Emotion, Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons, http://www.paulekman.com/wp-content/uploads/2013/07/Basic-Emotions.pdf 
  37. ^ Lazarus, Richard S.; Lazarus, Bernice N. (23 September 1996). Passion and Reason: Making Sense of Our Emotions. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780195104615. Retrieved 23 September 2017 – via Google Books.
  38. ^ "Emotional Competency – Recognize these emotions". Emotionalcompetency.com. Retrieved 23 September 2017.
  39. ^ Cowen, Alan S.; Keltner, Dacher (2017). "Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 114 (38): E7900–E7909. Bibcode:2017PNAS..114E7900C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1702247114. PMC 5617253. PMID 28874542.
  40. ^ "Psychologists Identify Twenty Seven Distinct Categories of Emotion – Psychology". Sci-news.com. Retrieved 23 November 2017.
  41. ^ "The Emotions Evoked by Video". Retrieved 2017-09-11.
  42. ^ Robinson, D. L. (2009). "Brain function, mental experience and personality" (PDF). The Netherlands Journal of Psychology. pp. 152–167.
  43. ^ "HUMAINE Emotion Annotation and Representation Language". Emotion-research.net. Archived from the original on April 11, 2008. Retrieved June 30, 2006.
  44. ^ Shaver, P.; Schwartz, J.; Kirson, D.; O'connor, C. (1987). "Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (6): 1061–86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061. PMID 3598857.
  45. ^ Parrott, W. (2001). Emotions in Social Psychology. Key Readings in Social Psychology. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0863776830.
  46. ^ "Basic Emotions—Plutchik". Personalityresearch.org. Retrieved 1 September 2017.
  47. ^ Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  48. ^ "Robert Plutchik's Psychoevolutionary Theory of Basic Emotions" (PDF). Adliterate.com. Retrieved 2017-06-05.
  49. ^ Jonathan Turner (1 June 2000). On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry Into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford University Press. p. 76. ISBN 978-0-8047-6436-0.
  50. ^ Atifa Athar; M. Saleem Khan; Khalil Ahmed; Aiesha Ahmed; Nida Anwar (June 2011). "A Fuzzy Inference System for Synergy Estimation of Simultaneous Emotion Dynamics in Agents". International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research. 2 (6).
  51. ^ أ ب TenHouten, Warren D. (1 December 2016). Alienation and Affect. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9781317678533. Retrieved 25 June 2019 – via Google Books.
  52. ^ Chorianopoulos, Konstantinos; Divitini, Monica; Hauge, Jannicke Baalsrud; Jaccheri, Letizia; Malaka, Rainer (24 September 2015). Entertainment Computing - ICEC 2015: 14th International Conference, ICEC 2015, Trondheim, Norway, September 29 - October 2, 2015, Proceedings. Springer. ISBN 9783319245898. Retrieved 25 June 2019 – via Google Books.
  53. ^ Plutchik, Robert (25 June 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. ISBN 9780819182869. Retrieved 25 June 2019 – via Google Books.
  54. ^ O'Shaughnessy, John (4 December 2012). Consumer Behaviour: Perspectives, Findings and Explanations. Macmillan International Higher Education. ISBN 9781137003782. Retrieved 25 June 2019 – via Google Books.
  55. ^ Plutchik, Robert (31 December 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. ISBN 9780819182869. Retrieved 31 December 2018 – via Google Books.
  56. ^ Izard, Carroll Ellis (31 December 1971). The face of emotion. Appleton-Century-Crofts. ISBN 9780390478313. Retrieved 31 December 2018 – via Google Books.
  57. ^ "The Nature of Emotions" (PDF). Emotionalcompetency.com. Retrieved 2017-09-16.
  58. ^ Plutchik, Robert (16 September 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. p. 110. ISBN 9780819182869. Retrieved 16 September 2017 – via Google Books.
  59. ^ TenHouten, Warren D. (23 June 2014). Emotion and Reason: Mind, Brain, and the Social Domains of Work and Love. Routledge. ISBN 9781317580614. Retrieved 10 December 2019 – via Google Books.
  60. ^ TenHouten, Warren D. (22 November 2006). A General Theory of Emotions and Social Life. Routledge. ISBN 9781134229079. Retrieved 10 December 2019 – via Google Books.
  61. ^ TenHouten, Warren D. (22 November 2006). A General Theory of Emotions and Social Life. Routledge. ISBN 9781134229086. Retrieved 10 December 2019 – via Google Books.
  62. ^ أ ب ت Kort, B.; Reilly, R.; Picard, R.W. (2001). "An affective model of interplay between emotions and learning: Reengineering educational pedagogy-building a learning companion". Proceedings IEEE International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies. pp. 43–46. doi:10.1109/ICALT.2001.943850. ISBN 0-7695-1013-2. S2CID 9573470 – via www.academia.edu.
  63. ^ Tiffany Watt Smith. "The Book of Human Emotions: An Encyclopedia of Feeling from Anger to Wanderlust" (PDF). Anarchiveforemotions.com. Archived from the original (PDF) on 18 April 2021. Retrieved 2017-05-28.
  64. ^ "Invisibilia: A Man Finds An Explosive Emotion Locked In A Word". NPR.org. Retrieved 2017-12-29.
  65. ^ "Happily disgusted? Scientists map facial expressions for 21 emotions". The Guardian. 31 March 2014.
  66. ^ Jacque Wilson (2014-04-04). "Happily disgusted? 15 new emotions ID'd". KSL.com. Retrieved 2017-07-16.
  67. ^ Design, Stamen. "The Ekmans' Atlas of Emotion". The Ekmans' Atlas of Emotions. Retrieved 31 December 2018.
  68. ^ "Atlas of Emotions > Stamen Design". Stamen Design. Retrieved 31 December 2018.
  69. ^ Randall, Kevin (6 May 2016). "Inner Peace? The Dalai Lama Made a Website for That". The New York Times. Retrieved 3 January 2019.
  70. ^ Evans-Martin, F. Fay (5 January 2019). Emotion and Stress. Infobase Publishing. ISBN 9781438119564 – via Google Books.
  71. ^ أ ب Reitsema, A.M. (2022). "Emotion dynamics in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic and descriptive review" (PDF). Emotion. 22 (2): 374–396. doi:10.1037/emo0000970. PMID 34843305. S2CID 244748515.


علم النفس

Psi2.svg

البوابة
تاريخ
علماء نفسانيون
مشروع
علم نفس بحثي أبحاث علم النفس الكمي · أبحاث علم النفس الكيفي · البيولوجي · Cognitive · المقارن · Developmental · Evolutionary · التجريبي · علم النفس العصبي · الشخصية · الفسيولوجي · الاجتماعي · الإيجابي · علم الأمراض النفسية · Psychophysics
علم نفس تطبيقي Assessment · السريري · إرشاد · التربوي · الشرعي / القانوني · Health · الصناعي والمؤسسي · Relationship counseling · المدرسي · الرياضي
مدارس واتجاهات Behaviorism · Cognitivism · Cognitive Behavioral · وجودي · Family Systems · Feminist · Gestalt · Humanistic · التحليل النفسي · التحليلي · Psychodynamic · Transpersonal
كتـّاب
مهمون
تاريخياً
ب.ف. سكنر · جان پياجيه · سيگموند فرويد · اوتو رانك · ألبرت بندورا · Leon Festinger · كارل روجرز · Stanley Schachter · نيل ميلر · إدوارد ثورندايك · ابراهام مازلو · Gordon Allport · إريك إريكسون · Hans Eysenck · وليام جيمس · داڤيد مكللاند · ريموند كاتل · جون ب. واطسون · كورت لوين · دونالد هب · جورج ميلر · كلارك هل · Jerome Kagan · كارل يونگ · إيڤان پاڤلوڤ
قوائم المواضيع · المجالات · الأدوية · الاضطرابات العصبية · المنظمات · أساليب البحث · مدارس النظريات · علماء النفس · العلاجات النفسية · المطبوعات · خط زمني