رؤية ثلاثية الألوان
Trichromacy or trichromatism is the possession of three independent channels for conveying color information, derived from the three different types of cone cells in the eye.[1] Organisms with trichromacy are called trichromats.
The normal explanation of trichromacy is that the organism's retina contains three types of color receptors (called cone cells in vertebrates) with different absorption spectra. In actuality, the number of such receptor types may be greater than three, since different types may be active at different light intensities. In vertebrates with three types of cone cells, at low light intensities the rod cells may contribute to color vision.
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Humans and other animals that are trichromats
Humans and some other mammals have evolved trichromacy based partly on pigments inherited from early vertebrates. In fish and birds, for example, four pigments are used for vision. These extra cone receptor visual pigments detect energy of other wavelengths, sometimes including ultraviolet. Eventually two of these pigments were lost (in placental mammals) and another was gained, resulting in trichromacy among some primates.[2] Humans and closely related primates are usually trichromats, as are some of the females of most species of New World monkeys, and both male and female howler monkeys.[3]
Recent research suggests that trichromacy may also be quite general among marsupials.[4] A study conducted regarding trichromacy in Australian marsupials suggests the medium wavelength sensitivity (MWS), cones of the honey possum (Tarsipes rostratus) and the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) are features coming from the inherited reptilian retinal arrangement. The possibility of trichromacy in marsupials potentially has another evolutionary basis than that of primates. Further biological and behavioural tests may verify if trichromacy is a common characteristic of marsupials.[2]
Most other mammals are currently thought to be dichromats, with only two types of cone (though limited trichromacy is possible at low light levels where the rods and cones are both active).[5] Most studies of carnivores, as of other mammals, reveal dichromacy; examples include the domestic dog, the ferret, and the spotted hyena.[6][7] Some species of insects (such as honeybees) are also trichromats, being sensitive to ultraviolet, blue and green instead of blue, green and red.[3]
Research indicates that trichromacy allows animals to distinguish brightly colored fruit and young leaves from other vegetation that is not beneficial to their survival.[8] Another theory is that detecting skin flushing and thereby mood may have influenced the development of primate trichromate vision. The color red also has other effects on primate and human behavior as discussed in the color psychology article.[9]
Types of cones specifically found in primates
Primates are the only known placental mammalian trichromats.[10][المصدر لا يؤكد ذلك] Their eyes include three different kinds of cones, each containing a different photopigment (opsin). Their peak sensitivities lie in the blue (short-wavelength S cones), green (medium-wavelength M cones) and yellow-green (long-wavelength L cones) regions of the color spectrum.[11] S cones make up 5–10% of the cones and form a regular mosaic. Special bipolar and ganglion cells pass those signals from S cones and there is evidence that they have a separate signal pathway through the thalamus to the visual cortex as well. On the other hand, the L and M cones are hard to distinguish by their shapes or other anatomical means – their opsins differ in only 15 out of 363 amino acids, so no one has yet succeeded in producing specific antibodies to them. But Mollon and Bowmaker[12] did find that L cones and M cones are randomly distributed and are in equal numbers.[13]
Mechanism of trichromatic color vision
قالب:Metamerism spectrum example.svg
Trichromatic color vision is the ability of humans and some other animals to see different colors, mediated by interactions among three types of color-sensing cone cells. The trichromatic color theory began in the 18th century, when Thomas Young proposed that color vision was a result of three different photoreceptor cells. From the middle of the 19th century, in his Treatise on Physiological Optics,[14][15] Hermann von Helmholtz later expanded on Young's ideas using color-matching experiments which showed that people with normal vision needed three wavelengths to create the normal range of colors. Physiological evidence for trichromatic theory was later given by Gunnar Svaetichin (1956).[16]
Each of the three types of cones in the retina of the eye contains a different type of photosensitive pigment, which is composed of a transmembrane protein called opsin and a light-sensitive molecule called 11-cis retinal. Each different pigment is especially sensitive to a certain wavelength of light (that is, the pigment is most likely to produce a cellular response when it is hit by a photon with the specific wavelength to which that pigment is most sensitive). The three types of cones are L, M, and S, which have pigments that respond best to light of long (especially 560 nm), medium (530 nm), and short (420 nm) wavelengths respectively.[17][18]
Since the likelihood of response of a given cone varies not only with the wavelength of the light that hits it but also with its intensity, the brain would not be able to discriminate different colors if it had input from only one type of cone. Thus, interaction between at least two types of cone is necessary to produce the ability to perceive color. With at least two types of cones, the brain can compare the signals from each type and determine both the intensity and color of the light. For example, moderate stimulation of a medium-wavelength cone cell could mean that it is being stimulated by very bright red (long-wavelength) light, or by not very intense yellowish-green light. But very bright red light would produce a stronger response from L cones than from M cones, while not very intense yellowish light would produce a stronger response from M cones than from other cones. Thus trichromatic color vision is accomplished by using combinations of cell responses.
It is estimated that the average human can distinguish up to ten million different colors.[19]
See also
- Visual system
- Monochromacy
- Dichromacy
- Tetrachromacy
- Pentachromacy
- Mantis shrimp (dodecachromats)
- Evolution of color vision in primates
- Young–Helmholtz theory
- LMS color space
References
- ^ "Color Glossary". Archived from the original on 4 أكتوبر 2015. Retrieved 8 نوفمبر 2006.
- ^ أ ب Arrese, Catherine; Thomas, Nathan; Beazley, Lyn; Shand, Julia (2002). "Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials". Current Biology. 12 (8): 657–660. Bibcode:2002CBio...12..657A. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00772-8. PMID 11967153. S2CID 14604695.
- ^ أ ب Rowe, Michael H (2002). "Trichromatic color vision in primates". News in Physiological Sciences. 17 (3): 93–98. doi:10.1152/nips.01376.2001. PMID 12021378. S2CID 15241669.
- ^ Arrese, CA; Oddy, AY; Runham, PB; Hart, NS; Shand, J; Hunt, DM (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 272 (1595): 791–796. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411.
- ^ Jacobs, Gerald H.; Nathans, Jeremy (2009). "The Evolution of Primate Color Vision". Scientific American. 300 (4): 56–63. Bibcode:2009SciAm.300d..56J. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0409-56 (inactive 2 أبريل 2024). ISSN 0036-8733. JSTOR 26001303. PMID 19363921.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of 2024 (link) - ^ Calderone, JB; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Spectral properties and retinal distribution of ferret cones" (PDF). Visual Neuroscience. 20 (1): 11–17. doi:10.1017/s0952523803201024. PMID 12699079. S2CID 10415194.
- ^ Calderone, JB; Reese, BE; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Topography of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta)". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 62 (4): 182–192. doi:10.1159/000073270. PMID 14573992. S2CID 9167855.
- ^ Sharpe LT, de Luca E, Hansen T, Jägle H, Gegenfurtner KR (2006). "Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy". Journal of Vision. 6 (3): 213–223. doi:10.1167/6.3.3. PMID 16643091.
- ^ Diana Widermann, Robert A. Barton, and Russel A. Hill. Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
- ^ Ronald G. Boothe (2002). Perception of the visual environment. Springer. p. 219. ISBN 978-0-387-98790-3.
- ^ Schnapf, J. L.; Kraft, T. W.; Baylor, D. A. (يناير 1987). "Spectral sensitivity of human cone photoreceptors". Nature. 325 (6103): 439–441. Bibcode:1987Natur.325..439S. doi:10.1038/325439a0. PMID 3808045. S2CID 11399054.
- ^ Mollon, J. D.; Bowmaker, J. K. (ديسمبر 1992). "The spatial arrangement of cones in the primate fovea". Nature. 360 (6405): 677–679. Bibcode:1992Natur.360..677M. doi:10.1038/360677a0. PMID 1465131. S2CID 4234999.
- ^ Wässle, Heinz (11 فبراير 1999). "Colour vision: A patchwork of cones". Nature. 397 (6719): 473–475. Bibcode:1999Natur.397..473W. doi:10.1038/17216. PMID 10028963. S2CID 4431471.
- ^ von Helmholtz, Hermann (1909). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik (3 ed.). Hamburg ; Leipzig: Leopold Voss. Retrieved 18 فبراير 2020.
- ^ von Helmholtz, Hermann (2013). Treatise on Physiological Optics. Courier Corporation. ISBN 978-0486174709. Retrieved 18 فبراير 2020.
- ^ Svaetichin, G (1956). "Spectral response curves from single cones". Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 39 (134): 17–46. PMID 13444020.
- ^ Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (2000). Principles of Neural Science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 182–185. ISBN 978-0-8385-7701-1.
- ^ Jacobs GH, Nathans J (مارس 2009). "Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution". Scientific American.
- ^ Leong, Jennifer. "Number of Colors Distinguishable by the Human Eye". hypertextbook. Retrieved 21 فبراير 2013.
External links
- The Straight Dope: "Are cats and dogs really color-blind? How do they know?"
- Svaetichin, Gunnar; MacNichol, Edward F. (نوفمبر 1958). "Retinal mechanisms for chromatic and achromatic vision". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 74 (2): 385–404. Bibcode:1959NYASA..74..385S. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1958.tb39560.x. PMID 13627867. S2CID 27130943.
- CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of 2024
- Articles with hatnote templates targeting a nonexistent page
- Missing redirects
- Short description is different from Wikidata
- Use American English from March 2021
- All Wikipedia articles written in American English
- Use dmy dates from March 2021
- مقالات ذات عبارات بحاجة لمصادر
- Color vision
- Visual system